Disciplines related to visual psychophysics and perception Speaker: Lothar Spillmann September 29, 2009
Visual psychophysics encompasses the beginning and the end of the visual processing
chain. This is a great opportunity to learn more about the disciplines that deal with the
intermediate stages. Let us start with the very beginning, dioptrics.
Here, you want to understand how an image forms on the retina by taking into
account the refraction of the cornea and lens (the clear window panes of the eye
through which the rays pass). An eye that is normally refracted is called emmetropic.
When the ocular refraction is insufficient because the eye is either too long or too
short, one speaks of shortsightedness (myopia) or farsightedness (hyperopia),
respectively. To correct for these conditions, you need to wear glasses (concave -,
convex +) to bring the focal point back onto the retina. Many Chinese and Japanese
people are myopic possibly from reading at short distance and under poor lighting
during childhood. Watch uncorrected people as they squint to improve their vision.
This reduces the size of their pupil aperture, thereby affording greater depth of focus.
The unit of refraction is diopter (1/ focal length in meters). For example, when your
near point is at 25 cm, your optician will prescribe a - 4 dptr lens (spherical
correction). If the meridians in a star figure do not appear equally sharp, then you will
require an additional correction for astigmatism (called a cylinder). You can find out
whether or not you have it by rotating your glasses by 90 deg. Without these
corrections, image quality will be suboptimal and the results of your experiment may
be affected. Setting up a Maxwellian View System requires superb knowledge of
physical and physiological optics. The classical work on the dioptrics of the human
eye was done by Hermann von Helmholtz and Allvar Gullstrand (who received the
To be able to specify the luminance, say, of a surface area, you need to measure it
with a photometer. Chromatic stimuli require a spectral photometer whose sensitivity
mimics the spectral sensitivity of the human eye in daylight (i.e., photopic). Such
meters are expensive, but are mandatory for stimulus specification. A unit of
measurement that is frequently used is candela per square meter (cd/m2). There are
many others that convert to each other. If you need to know the illumination on the
retina, you must take the size of the pupil into consideration (i.e., troland). The contrast of a periodical grating pattern on the monitor can be calculated from the
maximum and minimum luminances according to the Michelson equation: C = Lmax
- Lmin/Lmax + Lmin; contrast sensitivity is the reciprocal of this term.
Signal transduction in the photoreceptors
Light entering the eye and hitting the photoreceptors starts a cascade of isomerization.
This is the field of photochemistry, and it is a difficult one. Here it suffices to be able
to calculate the fraction of bleached rhodopsin (rod pigment) for stimuli of different
luminance and duration. William A. Rushton has provided easy formulas. A near 100
% bleach is seldom achieved and can take more than 1.5 hrs to recover until full
sensitivity has been regained. Be careful not to damage the eye. People who lack
vitamin A in their diet will gradually lose their ability to see at night. Make sure that
your subjects eat enough fish and carrots. George Wald received the Nobel Prize for
The retina is comparable to light-sensitive wallpaper at the back of the eye, but it is
more. It consists of a number of layers that process the stimulus before feeding it into
the optic nerve. These layers are the horizontal cells, which integrate signals from
many photoreceptors; bipolars, which interact with each other to set up lateral
inhibition for image sharpening; amacrine cells, which disperse the information again;
and, finally, retinal ganglion cells. Thus, there is no 1:1 telephone line from the
receptor level to the ganglion cell level (and onward), except in the fovea centralis,
where visual acuity is highest. Everywhere else, larger functional units are being
formed with large numbers of receptors, funneling their output onto one ganglion cell
(convergence); and many ganglion cells receiving their input from the same receptors
(divergence). Ramon y Cajal was awarded the Nobel Prize for unraveling the circuitry
The larger unit just mentioned is called the receptive field of a cell. It can be
compared with the visual field (or acceptance cone) of that neuron. A retinal receptive
field consists of a center and a concentric surround, which function antagonistically:
light falling onto the center excites the neuron, whereas light falling onto the surround
inhibits it (lateral inhibition). This type of cell is called an ON-center-neuron and
mediates a percept of “brighter”. In the opposite arrangement of the receptive field, all
signs are reversed: light inhibits the center, while it excites the surround (lateral
activation). This type of neuron is called an OFF-center neuron and mediates a
percept of “darker”. Typically, both kinds of neurons work at the same time,
determining the brightness that you see. Keffer Hartline received the Nobel Prize for his discovery of the receptive field structure of optic nerve fibers.
Note that the receptive fields of the retina increase in size from central to
peripheral vision. This explains the rapid decline of visual acuity as well as the
increase of sensitivity for light and motion towards the mid- (and outer) periphery.
Note that the density of cones is highest in the fovea, whereas the density of rods is
highest between 10 and 20 degrees. Larger receptive fields make use of spatial and temporal summation, summing the excitation by all the quanta falling onto a given
receptive field (5 arcmin2 to 1 deg2) within a given duration of time (approx. 100 ms).
In the compound eyes of insects, such as bees, receptive fields are invariant with
eccentricity. So is (I believe) their visual resolution.
Ophthalmological function tests
Assume you can no longer see as well as you used to and therefore consult an eye
doctor. He will first look at your fundus (retina) with a slit lamp and then check a
number of functions, such as the following:
Resolution of central vision is measured using an acuity chart (Landolt C’s, Snellen
optotypes, sinusoidal gratings); a value of 20/20 is considered normal. A lesser value
suggests short- or farsightedness or presbyopia. The latter typically begins at the age
of 45 years and results from a loss of refraction due to a smaller range of
accommodation of the lens (1 instead of 8 dptr). At a still higher age the lens may
become cloudy (incipient cataract) impairing perceived contrast and color vision. In
that case the eye doctor can no longer help by prescribing stronger glasses, but will
eventually consider an artificial lens implant. There can be more serious reasons such
as cell death due to a long-standing elevated pressure inside the eye (glaucoma) or
loss of photoreceptors and nerve cells due to old age (macular degeneration, see
below). Treatment of glaucoma should start as soon as possible and may require
life-long therapy (drops, pills, surgery) and constant monitoring.
The perception of color is typically ascertained by the use of the Ishihara
pseudoisochromatic color plates. These are carefully produced plates with numerous
dots of different color and size, which will reveal a number if read by a color-normal
person. However, for someone suffering from color anomaly or color blindness, it will
not be possible to see the (hidden) number, because all the dots have the same
brightness and therefore will not stand out. There are other tests to check color vision, such as the Munsell D15 or D100 tests which require sorting color chips of equal
brightness and saturation according to their hue. Here, different color defects yield
specific sorting patterns, which are used for diagnosis. A standard illumination is
recommended for this task. The oldest and most accurate method involves mixing red
and green in the Nagel anomaloscope to match unique yellow (575 nm). Color
blindness can be a reason for withdrawal of your driver’s license as you are liable to
misread the color of the traffic light (red is always on top, green on the bottom, with
yellow in the middle). It will also exclude you from several jobs where correct color
perception is needed. Did you know that Viagra will slightly change your color vision
In addition to the aforementioned procedures, your ophthalmologist may want to look
at the kinetics of your eye to adapt to various light levels, including complete
darkness. Imagine driving from the bright scene of a snowy day into the
semi-darkness of a tunnel, you will not see much for some 20 seconds, before you feel
comfortable. Lighting engineers attempt to compensate for this by a higher number of
lamps at the mouth of the tunnel with a gradual decrease towards the middle and end.
Conversely, when exiting from the tunnel it will take again a few seconds to
overcome the glare. These percepts occur even if one considers the fast response of
the pupil. When you track the progress of recovery by measuring the threshold as a
function of time after a bleach, one typically obtains a bipartite curve. The instrument
is called an adaptometer and the resulting curve is the dark adaptation curve. The
upper branch refers to cone vision (photopic) and represents high visual acuity and
color vision; it saturates at about 5 min and is followed by a lower branch referring to
rod vision (scotopic) that represents high sensitivity, but absence of color vision. This
latter branch reaches an asymptote as late as 45 min after the bleach. In a small range
in between these two, both kinds of photoreceptors work simultaneously (mesopic
vision). Light adaptation in the opposite direction proceeds much faster than dark
adaptation. The total span of sensitivity change due to photochemical adaptation is as
high as 7 log units (a factor of 10 million), enabling one to see the brightest as well as
the dimmest light. However, cat eyes and the eyes of an owl provide an even higher
sensitivity. These animals can see in the dark under condition, where you cannot.
In a small number of patients, the doctor may find an abnormal dark adaptation
curve. A shorter and elevated upper branch suggests reduced day vision, while a
shorter and elevated lower branch suggests reduced night vision. The first could be
due to cone dysfunction, resulting in lower visual acuity and affected color vision (a
relative central scotoma); the second might be due to rod dysfunction, causing a lower sensitivity in the dark and a smaller visual field (eventually approaching tunnel
vision). Both conditions are alarming when the doctor after more testing comes up
with the diagnosis: age-related macular degeneration (AMD) or retinitis pigmentosa
(RP). Very little can be done to stop these crippling diseases, although an early
diagnosis may help. Repeated laser coagulations and injections into the eye ball are
being used to prevent further loss of vision and ultimate blindness. There are heroic
attempts under way to return vision to such patients by slipping a thin photosensitive
foil underneath the retina to convert light into electric current and transmit the
resulting signals to the optic nerve fibers and visual brain. But results so far have been
controversial. There is a consolation: These two diseases, AMD and RP, afflict
predominantly older people (who in former times may have died before the disease
became manifest). The same holds for loss of eyesight from diabetes. Here the small
ocular vessels break, bleed into the vitreous body and deprive the retina oxygen and
nutrients. Fortunately, strict control of the amount of sugar in your blood can stave off
blindness for many years. Retinal detachment is an added complication in advancing
age, but may also occur in younger people. Flashes or dark spots are a telltale sign and
prompt immediate treatment by an experienced eye doctor.
A complete lack of cones is quite rare and is due to a hereditary disorder. Such
patients can read with high (12 x loupe) magnification, but have no color vision
whatsoever, and exhibit pendular nystagmus due to the lack of a fovea. They are
day-blind and avoid exposure to bright light by squinting and wearing dark glasses.
On the other hand, their night vision is superb, sometimes even better than in normals.
Read the chapter written by the best-researched rod monochromat Knut Nordby from
Oslo on his childhood and youth, and the book by Oliver Sacks on the Island of the
A systematic check of the patient’s visual fields belongs to the routines administered
by an eye doctor. Different kinds of manual and automated perimeters are available,
some requiring only a few min (central field), some much longer (central and
peripheral field), for an examination. As a rule, a small moving light spot is
introduced from the periphery of the visual field until the patient sees it. This is done
from all different directions (meridians), using test spots of different size, to establish
the boundaries of the visual field and Blind spot (kinetic perimetry). For a more
detailed examination., light spots of varying intensity (and sometimes color) are
briefly flashed in random locations of the visual field, and the absolute or differential
threshold is measured to establish the sensitivity profile across a given meridian
(static perimetry). Both procedures are invaluable functional tests to probe for ocular pathology, such as an enlarged Blind spot, an early sign of glaucoma. For reasons of
time, most practitioners rely on an abbreviated (inner) visual field plot, thereby
possible missing pathology in the peripheral retina. Ask your friend to map your own
visual fields and find out how much of it is binocular to provide depth perception.
In addition to the psychophysical tests, your eye doctor may recommend recording the
electrical currents generated by the retina in response to brief light flashes. This is an
objective test that gives information on the functional status of small hexagonal
sections of the retina when tested with light (no color, form, or motion involved). The
tested sections increase in size towards the periphery roughly in accordance with
receptive field size. Clinical diseases will show in a changed amplitude of the
Stereo-depth and contrast sensitivity in amblyopia
Until about 20 years ago, pediatric eye doctors used to see many children suffering
from poor visual acuity and contrast sensitivity due to squint (cross-eyedness,
strabismus). These children had inward or outward deviation of the optical axis of one
eye, producing two rivaling images in the brain. If this condition is not treated well
before entering school, by patching or surgery, amblyopia will result with a total and
permanent loss of stereo acuity (suppression amblyopia). This is because the brain
suppresses the weaker of the two images and as a consequence the binocular neurons
in the visual cortex will atrophy or be rededicated to some other function. Often the
contrast sensitivity of the amblyopic eye is affected together with stereo depth. Loss
of visual function in one eye can also arise from astigmatism (meridional amblyopia)
or unequal refraction of the two eyes (anisometropic amblyopia) in childhood. The
worst visual damage will result from a milky cornea (leucoma) or lens (cataract) at
birth as these conditions prevent clear image formation on the retina (deprivation
amblyopia). Surgery soon after birth is indicated in many instances to avoid blindness.
Such children will not be able to see when they are later operated upon, using a
corneal or lens transplant. In the absence of stereovision, amblyopic patients – as
one-eyed people - utilize secondary depth criteria such as apparent size, occlusion
cues, motion parallax, and perspective for gauging distance.
It is time now for you to change from your ophthalmologist to a cooperating
neurologist, for example, if you are suffering from migraine. Migraines are difficult to treat and are sometimes attributed to a hysterical personality (which makes the
problem even greater). Migraine scotomata have been tracked as they traverse the
visual field from the fovea to the periphery and have been interpreted in terms of a
slow electrical wave moving across the receptive fields of the visual cortex. These
zigzag or fortification phosphenes may reflect the activity of orientation-specific
neurons and have been used to infer receptive field size as a function of retinal
eccentricity. Other scotomata may be due to a tumor, gunshot wound, or hemorrhage
(stroke) and can render an entire quadrant or half of the visual field blind in one or
both eyes (hemianopia). From the presence of such deficits, the experienced physician
can tell approximately where in the visual system (below or above the optic chiasm)
the origin of the defect is located. Residual vision in and around scotomata from
cerebral trauma has been reported to slightly improve under a strict regimen of
rehabilitation. However, in Korean veterans who were followed up every 2 years over
a long period of time the gain was negligible.
Multiple sclerosis (MS) caused by plaque formation and demyelinization of the
nerve fibers in the optic nerve is yet another neurological syndrome by which vision is
affected. In MS-patients the conduction velocity of the two eyes may be differently
affected, producing temporal delays and a percept of two flashes instead of only one.
Such patients will see a pendulum that swings back and forth as rotating on an
elliptical path in depth, because the brain interprets the time delay in terms of lateral
disparity. This effect is known as the Pulfrich phenomenon and is used as a screening
test in multiple sclerosis together with visually evoked cortical potentials (VECP).
Challenges and opportunities in neuropsychology
The field of neuropsychology has been boosted by the advent of functional
neuroimaging (fMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Afflicted patients
can now be studied noninvasively without opening the skull. (You still need a
patient’s consent, of course, in line with the Declaration of Helsinki.) Such patients
may be seen as the result of an experiment by nature and offer unique insights into the
visual system. For example, using these techniques the precise location of deficits in
the living visual brain has become possible, such as cortical color blindness (cortical
achromatopsia) in area V4, disturbed motion perception (akinetopsia) in area V5/MT,
and face blindness (prosopagnosia) in the bilateral fusiform area. Disturbances of
form vision have been located in the inferotemporal lobe, whereas impairment of
spatial orientation has been attributed to the parietal lobe. These brain structures have
therefore been called the “what” and the “where” systems.
Neo-Gestalt-neuroscience The same techniques are being used to study brain architecture. Many of the visual
centers found by microelectrode studies in the cat and monkey are being confirmed
and refined. The cortical magnification factor (overrepresentation of the fovea on
the cerebral cortex) has been worked out in detailed studies, giving us a picture of a
geometric stimulus as it is mapped on the surface of the brain. More is to come. Basic
studies in conjunction with clinical studies help us to better understand vision and the
visual brain. I this endeavor, psychologists are often on the forefront because they
master more than one technique and also can put the individual findings from several
disciplines better together. We are living in an exciting time, comparable to the
beginning of single-cell neurophysiology in the early sixties that yielded an
unprecedented neuronal inventory of visual feature detectors for brightness, color,
orientation, motion, and depth (for which Hubel and Wiesel received the Nobel Prize).
This, in a way, is comparable to the advent of Gestalt psychology at the beginning of
the last century. It is to be expected that the laws of seeing, based on phenomenal
observations, can soon be correlated with neuronal processes and mechanisms. Would
you have imagined that advanced fMRI-analysis can reveal (an isomorphic replica) of
ARRHYTHMIA THERA The evidence on emergency treatment of arrhythmias was reviewed and the only change was the addition of procainamide as possible therapy for refractory supraventricular tachycardia (SVT). Unstable VT Consensus on Science There is insufficient evidence to support or refute the efficacy of electric therapy over drug therapy or the superiority of any drug for the emergenc
Asthma Medicine Medication Si usted desea esta información en español, por favor pídasela a su enfermero o doctor. Name of Child: ____________________________ Date: _________________ Asthma Medicine There are two types of asthma medicine: medicines that control asthma and medicines that rescue you from an asthma flare. Medicines to use every day - Controller Medicine